<<<


supplementary articles

header boat

2018 revision
a web page version of a pdf paper

NEW INTERPRETATION OF THE “URALIC LANGUAGE  FAMILY” LANGUAGES LANDSCAPE 

A large amount of  scientific information has accumulated in the last century that suggests a different narrative than the century-old one about the history of the northwest Eurasian languages

The science of historical linguistics analyzes surviving languages today,  to determine their relationships to one another, and  to reconstruct their evolution from proposed  earlier “proto” languages. The results may be presented in a tree diagram that describes a sequence of branchings from parental languages. But the interpreting of the linguistics is usually done by the linguists who then try to find support for their theory from other sciences like archeology and population genetics. Not only is this approach biased, with ‘proof’ being selective, but also linguists are not trained to understand those other sciences. For example they spout ‘evidence’ in population genetics, but have no understanding even of what population genetics does.  It would be like dance instructors, presuming to be able to diagnose physical ailments from information found on the internet.. Linguistic analysis and interpreting them in terms of actual geographical locations and historical events like migrations, are two separate things. Surely you need independent experts in the field, or at least a neutral third party educated adequately in all fields applicable. The interpretation of the indigenous languages of northwest Eurasia by 19th century linguists was done at a time when there was very little evidence yet from archeology and other sciences to assist in interpretation, and the linguists largely made it up using popular stereotypical  notions of the day. They populatized the common popular theory that “Uralic” languages  had a ‘tight’ origin near the Ural Mountains, and then expanded from there in a series of migrations radiating generally westward and giving rise to new languages. However this idea of a tight origin and migrations has never found support in the archeological information that has accumulated in the past century. Objections have been aired since the early 1900’s. Archeologists since the 1960’s pointed out the ideas from linguists did not have support in facts. Recently new population genetics informatrion suggests reindeer hunters from Asia introduced a second language source, and there was convergence, not divergence, at the start.  The following paper explores the accumulated evidence to see what model of linguistic evolution should be. This 2018 version of my interpretations  I believe is the best one, that fits all available information, including linguistic



1.

INTRODUCTION


THE TRADITIONAL “URALIC LANGUAGE FAMILY” – AN INVENTION OF A NAÏVE TIME, NEVER PROPERLY REVISITED OR REVISED.


For the last century, there has existed in the realm of historical linguistics, a model for the evolution of the indigenous languages between the Baltic and  the Ural Mountains and  beyond.  This model has been called “Uralic Language Family” created in the late 1800’s which forced the languages into a family tree.

 The traditional family tree model of the Uralic languages was first developed over a century ago by E. N. Setälä from an approach introduced by German philologist A. Schleicher. The traditional interpretation has been that there was a parent “Uralic” language at the base of the family tree, and that it produced breakaways who migrated westward and changed in a series of steps. But is am interpretation created over a century ago, when historical linguistics was new,  when little was known about the people who spoke the languages in early times, and when archeology and other sciences of the past were only starting?

 Today linguists know that languages do not evolved as simply as parental languages spawning daughter languages only by migrating and only by diverging. Today linguists know for example that areas of a single language can break up into dialects simply from internal changes that reduce communication, such as a widely distributed nomadic people settling down and becoming extremely localized, and that increased communication produces the reverse – different dialects or languages that can converge and produce similarities between such languages that look much like divergence from a single origin.

Today linguists are acknowledging truths such as dialects can converge if communication range increases again. For example the convergence caused by new national governments reducing many different dialects to converge to form a national language.

 The “Finnic languages” defined by linguists are basically recent reductions of numerous dialects in a continuum of a single language. That is to say: nearby neighbouring languages were dialects of each other, while distant neighbouring languages could be seen as only related languages relative to each other. But then the region was subdivided by invading powers and divided into large nations, forcing a continuum of dialects into a smaller number of official languages. Today’s definitions of languages do not reflect the natural situation that existed before the rise of civilization.

 Languages, we know, in the natural world, do not have those modern sharp boundaries created by organization into nations. Hard boundaries in early times were created only by sharp natural boundaries such as mountains (like Ural Mountains), large bodies of water (like Gulf of Finland) that prevented contact. In the original natural state, humans did naturally organize into families and tribes, so there were small dialectic groupingsl but in early humankind, there was no large scale heirarchical organization, unless there were real geographical shaping of patterns of contact and neighbourliness, such as boat peoples being confined to water systems, or reindeer people being tied to their reindeer herds and the lands used.

 In the light of what we know today, the early “Uralic Language Family” looks simple-minded, and the work done by linguists looks more like a puzzle game and not an investigation of the real world. 

Science by definition studies the real world. Historical linguistics was born when there was little to study – only some information from geography and anthropology.  But how can linguistics approach the subject of early languages in such a vaccuum. In the past century that vaccuu, has been filled by an enormous amount of information. We know the timing of the retreat of the Ice Age glaciers. We know how the “Ahrensburg” culture reindeer peoples faced a rapidly warming climate, and the disappearance of tundra and tundra reindeer, and humans were forced to hunt diverse animals of forests and marshes and develop dugout boats (the “Maglemose” culture) just to move around. We know how te “Swiderian” reindeer culture a little to the east around Poland was similarly compromised, but lasted a little longer in the northeast direction, before it too had to change to boat peoples (“Kunda” culture). We know how the world climate became as warm as it is today within 500 years, when the Ice Age glaciers still covered Scandinavia. We know how the warming culture caused a wildlife population explosion so that when the boat peoples mastered their skills for making dugouts and travelling in boats rather than on foor, they too had a population explosion and expanded. We know from sites along early rivers and bodies of water how the boat peoples expanded to the Urals. We know that there were people expanding north from Asia too, except their reindeer oriented way of life had greater longevity because the Ice Age glaciers did not block the tundra reindeer reaching the arctic, expecially at the Tamir Peninsula. We know that not all of these reindeer peoples managed to remain with reindeer herds and reach the arctic, and that the migrations of later stages when the world climate was as warm as today, could only stay with reindeer if the reindeer were semi-domesticated and driven up mountains in summer. We know from population genetics that there were reindeer peoples, or pedestrian ex-reindeer peoples in the Urals region who would have been quick to add to their way of life the innovation of the boat to get around faster than on foot, and to access the aquatic life – such as the Ob River basin wetlands. We know that obviously the European boat peoples expanding east, encountered the Asian reindeer peoples, or compromised reindeer peoples, who nonetheless carried the N-haplogroup in their men. We know how seasonally nomadic peoples move through the landscape in extended family groups, but gather usually annually at an established time and place to socialize, trade, find mates.(We know this from examples in Canada observed at European colonization in recent centuries.) We know that there would have been gathering places at the Urals that would have also drawn Asian reindeer peoples, and that it would have been in these gatherings that there would have been degrees of convergence between the two peoples at many levels – genetic, cultural, linguistic – and that the change caused by convergence would simply influence the genetic, cultural, and linguistic characteristics of peoples who did not make direct contact. We know today that language change only occurred by divergence as a result of migrations only in special circumstances when migrations took place and the migrants were too strong to become assimilated at their destinations, and that ever since humans inhabited the world after the Ice Age, language change was determined primarily by in situ divergence and convergence according to patterns of contact and interraction both socially and in large scale trade.

And there is much more that we know today, that was unknown a century ago when the established “Uralic Language Family” concept was invented.

 Perhaps  the linguists of a century ago did consult experts in various fields, but back then, in the late 1800’s until the mid-1900’s there was very little knowledge about the retreat of the Ice Age, expansion of humans into new ways of life when the climate warmed. All those early linguists had available to study was what was had been recorded by anthropologists who visited the speakers of the languages in question and the geographical locations. But today there is no excuse for creating or continuing to accept such a simplistic model when today so much new information has accumulated that – if analyzed in a multidisciplinary fashion – can reveal how the early past really happened, and what is the real evolution of the indigenous languages of northwest Eurasia today, the languages that the original theory labelled “Uralic”.



COMMON SENSE SUGGESTS MIXING OF TWO DIFFERENT CULTURES
 
Our subject is northwest Eurasia, beginning with the warming of the world climate and the  retreat of Ice Age glaciers towards Scandinavia where they had originated.

 Let us begin with common sense. Even without information from archeology and other sciences, it is obvious that when the Ice Age glaciers retreated back to the mountains of Norway, they left behind a flooded and depressed landscape in Europe south of what today are the North and Baltic Seas. Former tundra peoples who had moved around on foot, had to learn how to live and hunt in a land of seas, lakes, rivers, and marshes located south of the melting glaciers. They had to develop boats. Once they mastered a way of life using boats and hunting and gathering aquatic plants and animals, the continuing warming of the climate caused a population explosion. As tribes grew too large, breakaway groups moved away and formed new tribes. If they used boats their territories were large. Boats can travel great distances very quickly. And when these people had expanded out of the regions south of the Baltic, they found large rivers – notably the Volga, and rode that river towards the Urals very easily. Consider today taking a cruise down the Volga. It will take only some weeks. This means once the use of boats was mastered, groups could travel the length of the Volga within a summer. (In North America, canoe-using men of the Algonquian peoples were known to have travelled the length of the Great Lakes and back within a summer.)

 The fewer humans there were, the further people travelled. Their sense of scale was not measured by physical distance or time, but what you had to do to make contact with other peoples to satisfy the human need for social contact.

In the light of actual observed peoples living the way of life of the boat-oriented hunter-gatherers in northern forests, a better, more reality-based (less abstract and theoretical) approach was advanced already in 1907 by a Finn -  Heikki Ojansuu. Seeing how differences between the languages was generally in proportion to distances they were apart from each other, he did not see a family tree based on breakaways and migrations, but a natural language continuum, where languages developed in situ purely from divergence or convergence based on patterns of contact between such nomadic hunter-gatherers.   He wrote (using the “Finno-Ugric” term invented by Setälä)

“The Finno-Ugric peoples once occupied a broad zone extending somewhere from the region of Ilmajärvi, then along the Volga and its tributaries to the region of the Kama and the Urals”.

 He thought that  the original homeland must be thought of as a broad area not a narrow one, since hunters and fishermen need large areas for their activities. Next, another Finn, Paavo Ravila  pointed out that the geographical distribution of the languages appeared to closely reflected their relationships. Erkki Itkonen supported Ojansuu’s and Ravila’s views. Itkonen also explained how the large continuum of dialects developed into the many languages of today. As he put it:  when the once food-gathering peoples, who had originally needed wide areas in which to move about, became agriculturalists, and were more inclined to stay in one area, dialects became more and more separate and over the centuries and millennia developed into separate languages.

 Itkonen’s view then was essentially that during the time that the  hunters were very mobile and far ranging, their language was relatively uniform over the entire northwest Eurasia range. Then as they ceased their original mobile, far ranging, way of life, and settled in villages, they became more localized in their contacts and this large scale uniformity became broken up into dialects, extreme dialects becoming related languages. (Today, the grouping of languages into “Finnic”, “Volgaic”, “Permic”, “Ob-Ugrian”, suggests originally these language groups were only dialects of the boat-people language, and then when the nomadic way of life contracted, dialects developed into languages.)

That this is the correct view is today obvious.

But how did the original linguists get the story wrong?

When the Finno-Ugric family tree model was created in the late 1800’s, society was obsessed with genetic descent – this was the time when Darwin’s theory of human descent from apelike creatures was causing a great impact on the world.  Even though language is not a genetic trait, there was a strong inclination to treat is as if it was, with divergence of languages being akin to evolutionary mutation. Nationalism too, promoted treating languages as if they were genetic, and that continues today, as amateur linguists use archeological findings or the new information from Y-chromosome and mtDNA analysis to ‘prove’ a particular people spoke their language.

Sadly the original Setälä family tree model has held firm in spite of considerable opposition – increasing as information from other sciences accumulated. It is a reality that once an idea is popularized, it is difficult to remove, even if false. Defendants would rather come up with ridiculous expanations than allow the wrong idea to be replaced. For example, there has been the notion that the earth was at the center of the solar system and not the sun, or that the natives of North America all came over the Bering Strait land bridge (there is much evidence of many migrations, some coming by sea.).

The opposition to the simplistic, and unrealistic Setälä family tree model continued especially when archeology was revealing more applicable information. In the 1960’s  Estonian archeologist Richard Indreko pubished an article that discussed in detail the evidence of a general movement of material culture west to east out of northern  and not an east-to-west migration from any tight Ural Mountains origin.

Still, there was one aspect of the problem that tended to support notions of an eastern origin – the presence of peoples in the northwest Eurasian region with mongoloid physical features, suggesting influenced from mongolian peoples. In the northern direction, increasingly Finnic peoples show mongoloid characteristics in their faces – such as broad faces and high cheekbones. This suggests at least that there were mongoloid reindeer peoples across he arctic.  The presence of arctic reindeer peoples with mongoloid faces was a reality that any theory regarding northwest Eurasian peoples had to acknowlege. The original “Uralic Language Family” theory probably envisioned a mongoloid original peoples that divided between reindeer people (“Samoyeds”) and the boat peoples of the subarctic forests (“Finno-Ugrians”).  The latter, then, it was imagined, became Europoid in appearance from a more recent addition of European genes from the southwest direction.
It is probably this presence of mongoloid faces that have prevented the acceptance of a west-to-east expansion scenario as suggested by archeology. In the following pages, the obvious solution is that there were two sources – the migration from west to east revealed by archeology, and the migration from east to west in the arctic, revealed especially by N-haplogroup population genetics.

As we will see, the movement of the N-haplogroup in men north at the Urals and west across the arctic, is already suggested if we observe the locations of mongoloid characteristics – most frequent towards the arctic, and closer to the Yamal and Tamir Peninsulas.

It follows that the acceptance or rejection of a theory of the evolution of the indigenous languages of northwest Eurasia rests not on the expansion of cultures out of Europe, but of the expansion/migration of peoples of mongoloid origins.

While it has always been possible to take the original route of assuming an already mongoloid people divided between reindeer people and boat people, but over the past century archeology and other sciences have made events vivid. The expansion of peoples out of northern Europe via dugout canoes is clear. Therefore there must have been mongoloid peoples already in the regions into which the expansion occurred. The languages do not need to be the product of migrations but diffusion of influences from the reindeer peoples most strongly situated towards the Urals and the arctic.

Today population geneticists believe they have made a great discovery to find a diffusion of the Y-DNA N1c1-haplogroup generally from east to west. But this is already seen in a diffusion of mongoloid characteristics after some of the carriers of the N1c1-haplogroup changed their way of life to that of the boat peoples, and became part of a mixture of Europoid and Mongoloid characteristics from the Baltic to the Urals, the one stronger closer to Europe, the other stronger towards Siberia and the arctic.
   
WHY NOT A THEORY THAT BEGINS WITH THE MEETING OF THE TWO PEOPLES AT THE URALS?


The common sense observations thus show contact between two peoples. The original family tree theory was created before there was any knowledge of the development of boat peoples in the flooded lands south of the glaciers and their expanding east as far as the Urals. In Setälä’s time it was assumed the Europoid appearances in Estonians and Finns came more recently from Europe, from normal contacts. Archeological discoveries of “Maglemose” and “Kunda” boat-oriented cultures at the Baltic and their expansion already as much as 12,000 years ago (not just recently!) represent the greatest opposition against the traditonal “Uralic Languages Family” theory.  It clarifies the existence of TWO origins.

Instead of  Uralic origins among people who became both boat peoples and reindeer peoples, we have two separate peoples who come into contact with each other at the Urals, and then we have to consider what happened as a consequence.

If we were to continue to use a one-origins approach, we would have to go back to the Ice Age, and find a common language for ALL reindeer peoples (since the boat peoples arose from European reindeer peoples) But there isn’t enough evidence to claim Asian reindeer people and European reindeer people had a single original language. Thus we take the approach that there were two original languages that interracted at the Urals, and the modern languages developed as a consequence of this event.

Our  interpretation of events suggests there was a mixing of two cultures in the vicinity of the Urals and that was followed by a diffusion of the results to the original languages further away.

2.

THE  STORY OF THE MEETING OF TWO PEOPLES AT THE URAL MOUNTAINS

Figure 1





MEETING OF TWO DIFFERENT PEOPLES FROM TWO SOURCES AT THE URALS
, The map above (base map source given below the map) shows the circumstances at around 10,500 years ago. Most maps showing the retreat of glaciers fail to show the glacial lakes, and the manner in which the northern coasts were pushed considerably  southward by glacial meltwater. Although the “Maglemose” and “Kunda” material cultures arose from reindeer peoples (“Ahrensburg” and “Swiderian” cultures) the temperature was  as it is today, with glaciers melting and flooding the land. It should be clear from the map, that except for south Norway, and the British Isles, there was nowhere for European reindeer to go, but Asian reindeer  could find refuge in the northern Urals, or move northeast towards the ice-free northeast Siberian coast by which reindeer found refuge in the Tamir Peninsula and east along the arctic Siberian coast. Obviously the boat people (blue) and the Asian reindeer peoples, made contact at the Ural Mountains, and later in the northeast European arctic when it was possible for reindeer to migrate west towards northern Finland.

EVIDENCE FROM ARCHEOLOGY OF THE REINDEER PEOPLES AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF BOAT PEOPLES


As I say above, looking at Finnic peoples at the Baltic, one would see examples of mongoloid characteristics in faces of Finnic speakers, increasingly from the Finnish arctic southward. These characteristics came from immigrants from the east, in this case along the arctic coast, perhaps 5,000 years ago.

 Mongoloid characteristics can be seen as adaptations to arctic conditions – eyes to give a permanent squint against the glare of snow, broad flat face to create a pocket of air instead of cold wind moving around the face and removing heat. It can be argued that the longer a people remained in arctic conditions, the stronger these adaptations became. By this argument, reindeer hunting peoples were already slightly mongoloid in continental Europe, but the exaggeration of these traits would have continued with peoples who stayed in arctic conditions, by following reindeer herds north, as the climate warmed towards the south. In other words, reindeer peoples who followed the reindeer to the arctic from the arctic to today never left the Ice Age, and adaptations to arctic cold and white snow continued to be selected.  It follows, that if the boat peoples  originated from reindeer hunters in the North European Plain before about 12,000 years ago, ended their association with reindeer and remained behind in a climate similar to today, they would in general only show mild traits that adapted for arctic conditions. 

If we did not know about the migration of reindeer hunters from Asia, we might imagine that some European reindeer people reached the Tamir Peninsula area, millenia earlier than the rapid warming, and increased the mongoloid traits so that later, when they made contact again the physical trait differences had exaggerated. But the reality appears to be that the European reindeer hunters were unable to migrate north as a result of the glaciers and glacier lakes  preventing tundra reindeer from reaching the arctic and surviving. The barrier to northward shift of reindeer herds did not have to be visitble. In the depths of the Ice Age, where there were no glaciers, there was still polar cold where life was impossible. Thus even consideration of the region between the Baltic and Urals can only begin when the climate is warming and the tundra is moving northeast on the east side of the glacier edge. But then, as the climate warms, the melting ice causes glacial lakes that move the arctic coast southward eating up the advancing tundra, until there is no tundra. (See Figure 1)

No matter how you look at it, the European reindeer herds are blocked from moving north and staying in their arctic habitat, and that means sooner or later, all the European reindeer hunters have to convert to boat peoples, first into the “Maglemose” culture in southern Scandinavia south of the glacier edge, and then in the “Kunda” culture on the east Baltic coast east of the glacier edge. In fact, archeology believes from continuity of some artifacts that the “Kunda” boat using culture, developed out of the “Swiderian” reindeer culture.

Unless some survived in the mountains of Norway, we can confidently say that European tundra reindeer herds and reindeer hunters disappeared in Europe, the reindeer hunters converting to the boat-oriented hunter-gatherers, by about 10,000 years ago. That means the reindeer hunter component of indigenously peoples of northwest Eurasia arose from reindeer hunters of Asia, who were not blocked from northward shift because (see Figure 1) there were no glaciers  blocking reindeer progress, In fact the Tamir Peninsula became a refuge for tundra reindeer in the warmest times.

But before we consider the Asian reindeer people progress, let us look more closely at these people I refer to as “boat peoples” or “boat-oriented hunter-gatherers” who arose out of the compromised European reindeer hunters.


THE EMERGENCE AND EXPANSION OF THE MAGLEMOSE AND KUNDA BOAT PEOPLES – THE ARCHEOLOGICAL STORY



 Figure 1 depicts northwest Eurasia around 10,500 years ago, The world climate was as warm as it is today, and the glaciers were melting in torrents. The map shows how difficult it would have been to continue a way of life hunting reindeer in northeast Europe. The peoples of the flooded landscape released from under the glaciers (which I call “Uirala”) have been archeologically called the “Maglemose Culture” and “Kunda Culture”. Both emerged from about 12,000 years ago, the former from the “Ahrensburg Culture” of reindeer hunters, and the latter from the “Swiderian Culture” of reindeer hunters located before the accelerated climate warming in what is now Poland.

Archeology knew about the “Maglemose Culture” and the events that brought it about already in the 1960’s.  I quote from a famous archeological textbook:
… reindeer hunters of western and northern Europe during the period between ten and fifteen thousand years ago provide a well-documented example. Analysis of the larger game animals represented in the food-refuse of the Late-Magdalenians who sheltered in the south German cave of Petersfels for example, shows that they obtained four-fifth of their meat from reindeer. And even greater concentration can be seen on the summer hunting stations of the Hamburgian and Ahrensburgians sited on the margins of glacial tunnel-valleys in Schleswig-Holstein. In that case over 99 percent of the larger game animals were of a single species. The evidence suggests that other animals were the victims of chance encounters and that the only serious quarry was the reindeer...By attaching themselves to a herd of reindeer a group of hunters would not only possess themselves of a walking larder, comparable up to a point with a domesticated herd, but also a source of many of the most important raw materials they needed, skins for clothing and tents, antler and sinew for hunting gear. … quite suddenly, in the course of a few generations the ecological setting changed: as Late-glacial gave way to Post-glacial climate and glaciers entered on their final retreat, forests encroached rapidly on the open grazing grounds formerly occupied by reindeer. … the hunting people of the North European Plain reacted in part  by reverting to a mixed hunting economy ... but in part by developing special skills in fishing and winning food from the seashore.” (Clark 1967: 73–74.)

The archeological culture that arose from the Hamburgian and Ahrensburgian cultures was, as we mentioned earlier, called the Maglemose culture . The author continues:

The Neothermal inhabitants of this region [North European Plain most severely affected by environmental change at the close of the Pleistocene] had to adapt to a landscape transformed from park-like tundra into closed forest. ... People could no longer support themselves hunting a single species. ... Information is particularly rich in this respect of the Maglemosians who take their name from the big bog (magle mose) at Mullerup where their culture was first recognized. Their hunting grounds on the North European Plain extended in the west to eastern England and Flanders with outliers as far as Ulster and were centered on the marshy region now covered by the North Sea, and North German Plain, and the west Baltic area including Denmark and south Sweden; in the east they occupied parts of northern Russia as far as the Ural mountains. Over the whole of this territory they were fond of camping along river banks and lake shores on the margin of the encompassing forest, a favoured resort of certain game animals, including notably elk (= moose), as well as of wild-fowl, water-plants and fish.” (Clark 1967: 79.)

Knowledge about the expansion of the boat-oriented hunter-gatherers has of course been refined over the past decades, but the story is basically the same – an expansion of nomadic hunter-gatherers in a way of life involving northern forests and dugout canoes.

The boat peoples began with the “Maglemose” boat peoples at what is now Denmark and spread west to Britain and east to the Vistula and perhaps south through Vistula marshes. The “Kunda” culture arose from the “Swiderian” reindeer hunter culture located where Poland is today, probably drawing from both the “Swiderian” and “Maglemose” traditions. Archeology shows that the “Kunda” culture hunted seals and whales in the sea, which means they constructed large dugout canoes – probably for three pairs of oarsmen, and a helmsman, totalling seven people. With such large canoes, teams of men, or family units could make long journeys along seacoasts or large rivers like the Volga.

The expansion of the “Kunda Culture” as far as the Urals is suggested by archeology finding a region with “Kunda Culture” artifacts near the middle Urals, where the Dvina, Pechora, and Kama Rivers have their origins. Figure 2 next page represents a map of regions of archeological finds. In spite of different names and locations, on the ground they would all have been basically the same and spoken the same language.

The  map covers the results of the expansions of boat-oriented hunter-gatherers comprising events developing between  10,000-8,000 years ago; but our main interest is in the early general expansion of boat peoples towards the Urals.. The Upper Volga region represents one route of eastward expansion, while the Dvina represents another route of expansion and the route to the Urals for the Kunda Culture as the Dvina.

For further insight, I quote from Koslowski and Bandi. My underlining is added to notable portions.

A new wave appeared [in the Ural Mountains area]  only at the beginning of the Atlantic (period), in the upper Kama basin, and then advanced northward, reaching the Petchora and Vytchegda basins. This wave is represented by the Kama culture (Bader, 1966; Bourov, 1973)...”
This text continues to mention that artifacts associated with the Kunda Cculture that also reached the Pechora.
 “....The other (perhaps earlier) wave advanced from the western Russian plain across the Dvina basin, and is associated with the Kunda culture which represents the last descendants of the Swiderian. The two waves met in the Petchora basin, where the discoveries of Vis Pea Bog I, dated at 8080 +/- 90 yr and 7090 +/- 70 yr BP, give the most complete adaptation to taiga conditions, including many elements of the Kunda culture such as tangled points. Objects of wood and bone are preserved, including bows and arrows of wood. elements of skiis and sledges, bark receptacles and nets.

Figure 2

from Kozlowski J, and Bandi H-G  1984

The point is that the expansion of boat-oriented peoples eastward as far as the Urals by the major rivers is a certainty.The archeological story is clear – European boat peoples, originating from European reindeer people in Poland and Germany, expanded east as boat-using hunter-gatherers, from around 12,000 years ago.

Since, as Figure 1 shows, it was impossible for European reindeer hunters, such as the “Swiderian” reindeer people, to sustain their original way of life in northeast Europe, the only peoples in northeast Europe by 10,000 years ago, were the “Kunda” culture descended from the “Swiderian”. As boat people they took the Dvina to the middle Urals. We do not know if we should attribute the Kama and Volga cultures to “Maglemose” or “Kunda” initiatives.

In any case, boat peoples originating in continental Europe reach the Urals area and there a location in the middle Urals that was certainly a gathering place for both boat people and pedestrian hunter-gatherers from Asian origins living in those mountains and probably managing the reindeer in semi-domestication. Today still, there are reindeer people with domesticated reindeer, in the mountains of southern Siberia and northern Mongolia, who take herds up the mountains in summer and down in winter. Perhaps this was already done 10,000 years ago, in the Urals to the northwest, and the practice moved north through the Urals. The next sections will investigate the story of Asian reindeer people in the Urals.

REINDEER PEOPLE IN THE  URALS: THE ARCHEOLOGICAL STORY

Archeological investigations done in the Ural Mountains has proven that there were hunters in the Urals already in the Ice Age. Archeology shows that during the Ice Age there were wholly animals in the arctic tundra of the time and they were pursued by hunters.

I refer  to the study of the prehistoric events at the Ural Mountains presented in Kozlowski J, and Bandi H-G  1984  The Paleohistory of Circumpolar Arctic Colonization. According to this resource, the story of reindeer peoples in the Urals begins with the “Kostienki-Sungir” culture at the beginning of the Upper Paleolithic (40,000 BP to 10,000 BP). By about 25,000 BP (Before Present) this culture occupied “the most northerly location among lithic industries of the Upper Paleolithic”.  This culture is most famous for a site near Vladimir, Russia. This site revealed these people lived mainly on reindeer, mammoths, and horses. There was tundra there, and dwellings were constructed of mammoth bones. This early culture reached the northern Urals, and artifacts there have been radio-carbon dated to about 18,320 +/- 280 BP.

Figure 3



from Kozlowski J, and Bandi H-G  1984

UPPER PALEOLITHIC (40,000-10,000 BP) ARCHEOLOGICAL SITES
Upper Paleolithing refers to  the period from 40,000 years ago to 10,000 years ago. It is interesting to note that there were arctic hunter peoples expanding north from both Europe and Asia already an earlier time, before the great metling that gave rise to boat peoples (Maglemose and Kunda) This map shows in the solid arrow lines and light blue,  the way the glacier’s edge directed European Ice Age tundra hunters northeast. Were they related to the Swinderian culture? They were European reindeer hunters, but, as described in the last section, they did not last.  But note the dashed arrows coming up the Urals from Asia.

In the map of Figure 3, the solid arrow running parallel to the eastern edge of the glaciers, were tundra hunters from continental Europe, whose northward travel was directed by the edge of the glaciers. The starred numbers 1,2, and 3, called the “Kostienki-Sungir” culture were probably following migratory tundra animals, and the more northerly archeological finds at 2, may represent a summer location for an annually migrating people. Most other sites shown in Figure 3, are considered expansions of the “Kostienki-Sungir” culture. In archeological jargon, these peoples were in the Urals in the “Interpleniglacial phase” which was followed by the “Tardiglacial phase”.

The Tardiglacial phase cultures appear to represent the full conversion to reindeer hunting. Mammoths were disappearing, and nobody really knows why. It could be that, unlike reindeer, who had an instinct for migrating north south by more or less the same paths for generations, the mammoths may have been slow wanderers who were compromised by rapid climate change. According to Kozlowski and Bandi, the Tardiglacial phase artifacts had a style suggesting it had arisen from the Magdalenian reindeer cultures of Europe. In general, all the cultures across he North European Plain and into Poland and Russian Plains were then primarily reindeer hunting peoples with a reindeer-hunting culture descended from the Magdalenian culture of western Europe.

Kozlowski and Bandi acknowledge the northwards shifting of the reindeer cultures with the climate warming. All these reindeer cultures “followed the northward movement of the periglacial environment during the retreat of the Ice Age” This states the obvious – as the climate warmed, the open tundra shifted north, and the tundra reindeer herds shifted with the tundra – until unable to do so any further, of course. Thus the “Tardiglacial” period allowed the reindeer people to continue their way of life, and simple shift north with the reindeer, except where northward shifting of reindeer was blocked by glaciers or glacial lakes.

There is no proof that Tardiglacial colonization of the northern Russian Plain and the Urals lasted until the beginning of the Holocene.” (Holocene refers to the period when the world climate was as warm or warmer than today, and which is the time of the drama between boat people from Europe and a different reindeer people from Asia)

Here the authors confirm that former tundra animals throughout the northern Russian Plain for the most part could no longer survive – animals like the wholly mammoth, the wholly rhinocerous and other animals that had adapted to arctic cold – and that the ‘colonization’ of the north Russia Plain and the Urals did not last. This is obvous from Figure 1. All the European reindeer hunters converted into “Kunda” culture (and descendant cultures of “Maglemose”) which was much better adapted to the lands and waters of northwest Eurasia.


CONFIRMING DEPENDENCE ON REINDEER IN THE URALS

Archeology of the Ural Mountains covers a large period of time, while we are only interested in the last period, close to 10,000 years ago, after the warming had dramatically changed the climate landscape and boat-oriented hunter-gatherers were appearing at the Urals from the west. Who were the people there in the middle Urals time, and what were they hunting? Were they mainly dependent on reindeer?

Archeological sites are investigated according to strata of layers of dirt buildup. The most recent occupation of the site is near the surface, and older occupations are at deeper levels. What we are interested in is animal bones that reveal what the people hunted. Does it show an increase in reindeer hunting over time, and did archeology find that the latest stratum contained mostly reindeer bones? 

The earlier bones found by archeologists are typical of the Ice Age and included mammoths, rhinocerous, reindeer, grouse and bison. The northern sites are those marked 6 known as Medveja Cave, and 5 (Krutaya). Here we see evidence of a decline in mammoths and, over time, a greater dependence on reindeer.

The dating of these two sites is problematic: pollen analysis of the sediments of the Medveja Cave (lower level) indicates absence of elements typical of the tundra, and the presence of pollens more characteristic of a steppe environment. Among the fauna, reindeer predominates (>20%), followed by hare.....”

The authors give a table for animal bones found at the Medveja cave, where in the lower (older) layer there are 2271 reindeer bones and 2304 hare, and 3102 grouse. Bones of large animals other than reindeer are less than 10%  of the reindeer numbers. Or said in another way, in terms of larger animals, reindeer bones are 10  times more abundant than other animal bones.  Such large numbers suggest they were reindeer people – killing large numbers at a time by intercepting them in their migrations. Or else they were semi-domesticated already, and under human management. This is from the earlier period  but it proves that there were reindeer peoples in the Urals in earlier times. But were they still there at the time of the arrival of the Post-Swiderian boat peoples coming from the east via the Volga, Kama, Dvina,and Pechora?
 
But let us inspect the yet next, more recent layer  Reindeer bones appear still  very high at 1282. Actual numbers are not relevant, compared to relative numbers compared to other animals, since we may be only speaking of a smaller population of people, who ate less..
  I ask the reader to locate the Medveya cave site on the map of Figure 3.  It is located at the triangle with number 6. Note that it is located at the end of the Pechora River, where it touches the Ural Mountains.  We can propose this was the major location of contact between the boat people and Ural Mountain reindeer people. Was it a major annual gathering place of boat people tribes in that location that included reindeer people of Asian origin?


5

Figure 4
A NATURAL MEETING PLACE AT MIDDLE URALS

 Closeup of the location where the Dvina, Pechora, Kama water basins came close together and also close to the Ural Mountains in a location with relatively high mountains, so that technically there could have been reindeer people with semi-domesticated herds at that location around 10,000 years ago. Red triangles with elevations mark locations of higher mountains.  These are comparable to some mountains in south-central Norway. It is possible the reindeer may have been managed –guided up mountains in summer, and down in winter. The blue arrows show access by boat peoples from the Dvina, Pechora, and Kama water basins. as well as possibly the Ob. The location of the Medveja Cave archeological site (#6 in Figure 3) is within the light purple circle.

REINDEER PEOPLE IN THE  URALS: THE GENETIC  STORY

The archeological data confirms there were people in the middle Urals who consumed mostly reindeer and hares, at a location close to the sources of three or four rivers. It is definely a candidate for a major gathering place of tribes, where boat peoples made regular contact with Urals reindeer peoples.

The new field of population genetics reveals more.

Population genetics has identified Y-DNA N-haplogroups in male populations in northern and arctic Eurasia associated with reindeer peoples. In spite of the challenges of interpreting population genetics data, in the case of the Y-DNA N-haplogroups, the story is quite clear. The fact that the highest frequencies of the N-haplogroup are found today among peoples associated with reindeer across arctic Eurasia – almost all Samoyedic men in the Tamir Peninsula carrying it – seems to show clearly that the N-haplogroup was carried north along with the reindeer herds.

During climate warming, reindeer had to migrate north to stay within their tundra habitat, and if the reindeer herds had tribes who were dependent on them, those tribes had to follow them. The climate warming would have been a slow process, little by little over the period of a millenium or so, initially, so that neither the reindeer nor its human followers would have been aware of their geographic shift.

However, as we saw in the descriptions earlier of the climate warming in southern Scandinavia, the climate warming accelerated, and it is easy to imagine the animals and humans slower to respond would have been in great difficulty. Reindeer people too far south when the climate warming accelerated would have had to change their way of life to include other animals, and if they encountered the European boat peoples appearing from the west, they would have added innovations in their way of life to their own – notably the advantages of having boats and travelling longer distances at a faster rate through waterways.

By way of background, Y-DNA haplogroups are markers in sexual DNA that are passed down from fathers to sons for hundreds of generations, without being broken apart nor recombined with mother’s DNA as occurs normally. Since humans, like our related great apes, form social groups (bands, tribes) that are defined and defended by males, the apparent migration of the male DNA haplogroups reflects the migration of the tribes. (Female DNA creates a picture that is more blurred, because females are brought into the male-managed society from outside, from neighbouring bands/tribes.)

When we speak of peoples dependent on reindeer, their tribes will tend to consider specific reindeer herds as their territories – hence repelling rival tribes from accessing those herds without permission. Since reindeer people were intimately connected with the herds they considered their’s, when the world climate warmed and the herds shifted north to stay in their preferred cool climate, these tribes followed. Therefore the observed northward migration of the N-haplogroup north through the Central Siberian Plateau or the Ural Mountains, represents tribes following reindeer herds to which they were connected.

Discoveries were made that suggested the N-haplogroups in general originated in southeast Asia around 20,000 years ago, and gradually migrated north, in conjunction with the climate warming as the Ice Age began retreating.

Halogroups mutate, and major mutations that occur only after many millenia, are identified with capital letters of the alphabet. Lesser mutations are identified with numbers and small letters. Originally the N-haplogroup was divided between N2 and N3. The N2-haplogroup had the highest frequency in the Ngasan men of the Tamir Peninsula, and it suggests there was an earlier migration with reindeer herds north through the central Siberian Plateau, that reached the vicinity of the Tamir Peninsula that was not affected by the Ice Age glaciers. The N3-haplogroup represents a slightly later wave of northward migration, perhaps bordering on being late. I personally suspect that this second group had developed some skills in managing the reindeer herds (semi-domestication), which means to help the reindeer cope with the warmer climate, to drive the herds up mountain slopes in summer, and back down in winter. The reason I believe that some degree of management was in effect is because there are still today reindeer managing people in mountains of northern Mongolia or southern Siberia, who practice driving reindeer herds up mountains, and even ensuring they find locations here they can find the food they require.

The N3-haplogroup, which has more recently been renamed N1c1, was determined by a detailed investigation by Rootsi et al (2006) to have been at the southeast of the Ural Mountains at about 12,000 years ago. Since this N3 (N1c1) haplogroup ended up in signficant densities both in men in northern Finland in the west arctic and in northeast Siberia among Yakut men, Rootsi et al, suggested the group divided in two – one branch travelling northeast through the Central Siberian Plateau and ending up in northeast Siberia and the other branch travelling northwest probaby through the Ural Mountains, eventually reaching northern Finland. The Rootsi et al. investigation concluded that the N1c1-haplogroup migrated first up the Ural Mountains reaching the arctic by 10,000 years ago, and then when the environment made it possible, went west along the arctic coast.

As I mentioned earlier, just as one can see mongoloid characteristics in faces of indigenous peoples of northwest Eurasia, so too population geneticists will find the N1c1-haplogroup. Obviously what has happened is that through intermarriage, the Asian reindeer people genes have been passed into descendants who no longer follow a reindeer-oriented way of life. This migration of the N1c1-haplogroup could have begun already before 10,000 years ago, when some of the more southerly reindeer peoples were unable to sustain their reindeer-oriented way of life, and abandoned it early, beginning with adding the use of boats and accessing aquatic animals in a new way of life. In effect these people became part of the European boat peoples, and their male descendants propogated the N-haplogroups forward, mixing it into the genetics of the original European boat peoples over the following thousands of years.

Population genetics should note that this is not the result of single migrations, but in general a diffusion. The diffusion would follow the Kama to the Volga and the Volga to the Baltic for one simple reason – they used boats and boats travelled up and down the Volga, especially after the development of the fur trade around 5,000 years ago. The second diffusion came south from northern Finland. There too the fur trade may have been responsible. Furs were taken south via the Dneiper, and if there is a higher frequency of N1c1 in northern Poland, that is because the major fur trade center of the Baltic was located at the southeast Baltic where Elblag is today. Furs originally went south via the Vistula, and then either continuing down the Vistula or transferring to the upper Oder.
 .

Figure 5
This is a section of one example of population genetics plotting of percentages of N3 (N1c1) haplogroup.  The high concentrations (40-50%) in the Pechora River basin suggests a substantial portion of the Ural Mountains reindeer peoples, departed from their reindeer-based way of life there. This is predictable from the meeting place shown in Figure 4. Some pure reindeer people must have  remained, and later migrated to northern Scandinavia since a very strong reindeer culture survives in the Saami and diffused south from there..
The plotting also shows how the N1c1 haplogroup appears to have diffused south into the Finnic east Baltic. This diffusion is consistent with expansion of trade, and roughly agrees with the distribution of the archeological “Comb-ceramic” culture.
(The numbers represent percentages of the N1c1 haplogroup in male populations. the lines and shadings try to group them. Note almost 50% in the Pechora and up to 70% in Finland.)


3.

THE EVOLUTION OF LANGUAGES ACCORDING TO THE ARCHEOLOGICAL AND POPULATION GENETICS INFO

THE LARGE SCALE OF A SINGLE LANGUAGE CONTINUUM IN NORTHERN PEOPLES

Today our languages cover vast regions first because of the creation of large nations with imposed national languages and now because of mass media crossing national boundaries. But originally languages were maintained by natural patterns of human contact.

Originally, the scale over which there existed a single language depended on the scale of human movements over their landscape. The scale of human movements was dependent on density of food sources which depended on climate and landscape. Food density was high in jungles, and tribes there did not have to travel far to find their food, but food density approached zero towards the arctic and tribes had to be steadily nomadic to cover vast territories mainly to hunt.

If nomadic boat using hunter gatherer tribes annually covered regions hundreds of kilometers In diameter, or elongated further along water courses or coasts, and the met neighbouring tribes regularly – annually or every several years, at established gathering places to socialize, share, find mates – then a single language could be established over a vast area.  Such nomadic boat-oriented people were found among the Algonquian speaking tribes of the east half of Canada (as Europeans found in the 17th century), and the language over that vast area varied only dialectically. There was a linguistic continuum where closest neighbouring tribes could communicate with each other well, while distant neighbours could communicate with difficulty.

In the whole Algonquian language family, which covered the entire east half of what is now Canada, the most northerly language – Cree – covered the entire lower Hudson Bay water basin, with only three dialectic variations associated with three major rivers. These peoples travelled up and down these large water systems in birchbark canoes, following the pattern of extended family units ‘owning’ branches of the water system, and some 5-7 such extended families, annually gathering as a tribe at the mouth. (The reason why the Hudson Bay trader company placed trading posts at the mouth was to be where the families gathered).

At the upper ends of the rivers, the Cree made contact now and then with Ojibwa tribes travelling rivers draining into Lake Superior and Lake Huron. The Ojibwa language of Lake Superior and Lake Huron was dialectically different from Cree because of the simple fact that boats did not easily move through watersheds, and also because social contact was in another direction. Still, the Ojibwa language, covering the  Great Lakes water basin, with its own small dialectic variations, was close enough to also be able to communicate with the Cree. I have learned that today in the town of Sioux Lookout, located in the watershed area between the Lake Superior water basin and the Hudson Bay water basin, the Cree there make fun of some Ojibwa words and expressions, and Ojibwa make fun of Cree. Because of the mixing of both peoples in that city, apparently there has developed a hybrid language called “Ojicree” based on what is in common with both languages. .

Moving east among the Algonquian boat peoples, the dialects changed according to  water basins, generally defined by water basins which defined the tribes.  (See Appendix 1 for more.)  East of the Great Lakes Ojibwa we could technically consider the Ottawa River basin peoples to belong to a distinct dialectic region, next, the Sauguenay River basin in Quebec, and next the Churchill River basin in Labrador.
There were Algonquian peoples to the south too – the Miqmaq in Nova Scotia, the Maliseet in New Brunswick (traditionally people of the Saint John River), and a few more south into the States. The further south these people, the more dense the availability of food from the wild,  the reduced scale of their tribe definition,  and the rate of variaton in the dialectic continuum.

The Algonquian canoe-oriented hunter gatherers are the perfect model for the Ur-Finnic boat peoples who expanded east from the Baltic region at the end of the Ice Age in Europe. Everything is the same – the same post glacial flooded landscape, the same dependence on boats, the same seasonal nomadic way of life, the same gathering of extended families annually, large scale organization according to the heirarchy of water systems, and not artificial political organization, and above all, the large scale continuum of language, where closest neighbours are dialects to each other, and only the most distant (like Ojibwa relative to Labrador Innu) are different enough to be regarded as related languages to each other.
 
But why did recent Algonquian tribes not show the developments seen in the Eurasian boat people descendants, where the most westerly Finns are thoroughly modern.and in the remote northeast there are some groups who still live in traditional ways resembling Cree of about 50 years ago.  The answer is obvious, the Ur-Finnic peoples have been subjected to the forces and innovations of civilization for millenia, and the amount of change is proportional to the amount of contact and influence from civilization.
Figure 6


Late Ice Age in Ice America shows how a flooded landscape appeared in southern Quebec and Ontario into which a boat-oriented boat people could spread into.

The Algonquian boat peoples may have similarly originated when glaciers melted and created a flooded landscape around 10,000-8,000 years ago. But they did not change because for all those millenia they escaped being impacted by civilization or even by native cultures to their south. North America at the time of European contact was situated at approximately the European copper age – people used stone tools but valued metals that occurred in usable form in nature like copper, gold, silver.  Long distance trade was beginning up and down the Mississippi River and because Mississippi is the Algonquian word meaning ‘river’ boat people became long distance traders, just like in Eurasia on rivers like the Volga, around 5,000 years ago..

. In early humankind, northern peoples will have single languages covering thousands of km, while peoples in southern jungles will exist in such a high density that there will be a different tribe, culture, and language every few km, and all will be polarized and warring with each other. 

This suggests during the Ice Age there were few languages, maybe a continuum of a single language with dialect changing with distance between two locations. This supports the notion that the Ice Age reindeer peoples had basically the same language across all reindeer hunting peoples with even a small amount of contact. Direct contact with distant locations was not necessary. The convergence can pass between immediate neighbouring tribes who then influence the next tribes, until some 6,000 km of reindeer peoples are covered with the amount of linguistic change between the two ends being no more than that of related languages.  This would not have been true further south, where the climate was warmer and food density higher. However we are here interested in the northern peoples.

In the north, as the world climate warmed, the wide distribution of reindeer people  MAY have been speaking a single reindeer people language, and a broad origins “Tundric” language. With the end of tundra and flooding of lands, the reindeer peoples of Europe, evolved into the boat peoples, who expanded eastward. Their language which I call “Ur-Finnic” expanded as far as the Urals, were it made contact with the Asian reindeer people whose original language, I will call “Ur-Samoyedic”


THE CONTRACTION OF SCALE AND INCREASE IN DIALECTIC DIVERGENCE
 
Unlike the Algonquians in North America, the original Ur-Finnic boat peoples came under influences from southern civilization, beginning with the fur trade. This made indirect contact with southern civilizations and an increased in awareness of innovations. The creation of settlements and praticing farming could be attempted where feasible. But in general, additional ways of life were possible other than the traditional seasonal nomadism harvesting the natural wilderness over wide areas of forests and marshes.

We thus see in the history of the peoples who inhabited the regions from the Baltic to the Urals, a contraction of the scale of contact and interraction with neighbouring peoples. This caused dialectic divergence to become stronger, and additional smaller regions of dialects to develop. The final result is what we find today – several groups of languages, one for each major water system between the Baltic and the Urals, each containing several languages/dialects.

It is easy to understand the reduction of scale, and the associated increase in dialectic divergence, progressed only gradually. What we see today may be the product of developments only in the last millenia or two! It is possible to imagine for example that the entire east Baltic coast was, around two thousand years ago, a single continuum of dialects covering the entire east Baltic coast and eastward as far as Lake Onega. But since then, from establishing of farming settlements, foreign powers carving up lands and imposing their own languages on natives, and the instituting of modern political nations, many Finnic languages are now defined but more or less artificially. The further we move from the impacts of civilization,  such as remote parts of northwest Siberia, the more the situation is closer to an original situation.

Let us compare it with Algonquians of North America. There we find parallels. They were impacted in a major way from European colonization. First they were drawn into the European fur trade. Only in the last centuries has there been pressures to settle down (cease their seasonally nomadic life)  and to pursue economic practices (farming, industry, etc) of the European colonists. In addition now there is mass media. All peoples everywhere now are able to participate in the new worldwide media culture. We are all converging on the culture and language dominating the media world today.

Before mass media or even earlier governments imposing a national language, contact was dependent on actual physical contact. Even little contact was a great deal if the people were starved for social contact. As demonstrated by the Algonquian traditions, extended families would have gathered annually to define their tribe, and neighbouring tribes could visit as well. Such behaviour is natural. We see it through the ages in large human gatherings. All peoples who travelled hundreds of kilometers to meet at such places, in effect maintained the common language of all peoples participating. And if some tribes visited gathering places of more distant tribes as well, the  language went even beyond the range of individual tribes.

IN PRACTICE, HOW LANGUAGE IS MAINTAINED OVER A WIDE REGION

 Except for overpopulated circumstances that promote polarization, humans in their natural state want to socialize with other humans, and do so at several natural levels – the extended family unit, the natural tribe that is an association of some 5-7 extended families, and a sense of belonging to a ‘people’ consisting of several neighbouring tribes. For example speakng of boat peoples, extended families are defined by branches of a large river, the tribe of extended famiies is defined by the river as a whole, and the ‘people’ is defined by the tribes interconnected by water on a larger scale. There are other ways in which humans become naturally organized in the geography.

 Beyond this there is an awareness of a larger humankind, and even animal-kind. No direct contact is necessary. Information coming through news and gossip is enough to develop a sense of belonging to humankind as a whole. Today we watch the news, and develop a collective view of the circumstances of humankind as a whole.
For example, during the time of ancient Greece, northern Europe was aware of exotic cultures in the south, and ancient Greece was aware of fantastic peoples ‘beyond the North Wind’ (“Hyperboreans”).

Humans need to imagine something for the larger universe. The smallest evidence of humankind beyond reach was enough to fire up the imagination and an insatiable interest to know more. Long distance traders were celebrities, wined and dined when they returned from afar, to query them of the distant peoples. There was always a fierce desire to identify with the larger humanity, that was frustrated by the difficulty of ordinary people to make contact. This is natural in humans, and manifest today in our modern media-defined humankind. Any crumb of information about some remarkable thing far away, fires up a quest to learn more.

  Even if there is only a little contact, we will act to resist divergence. For example when immigrants from Europe established colonies in North America, and the only contact between the colonies and their European homeland was one ship a year, every measure was taken to prevent the language used in the American colony from deviating from the original language in Europe. Before mass media, one way of preventing divergence was to entertain a strong ideal of ‘speaking properly’. Elders would always have a sense of the proper way of speaking the language, and insisting their young people spoke in this proper way, and did not stray into their own inventions and slang.

To be specific, the city of Boston in America was one of the first British colonies established – in the 1500’s, at the time of Elizabethan Britain, at the time of William Shakespeare. Colonies were modelled after towns in their homeland, and the town would look like a town back in Europe. If one had no reminder that their homeland was thousands of kilometers away across an ocean, one could live and work in the colony as if the rest of their people were only kilometers away not across an ocean.
Because contact consisted of perhaps one or two ships a year, a North American colony paid a great deal of attention to information about their original homeland. The practice of sending letters back and forth was helpful, even if it took a month or more for a letter to cross, and a reply might only arrive a year later. There was also information in books and newspapers coming across – the beginning of mass media. The result was that Boston resisted change even as much change was taking place at the original country, Britain. While books and letters were easily carried across on ships, there was no good way of communicating changes in the way English was spoken back at home. Today scholars have studied the well-established Boston accent, and concluded that it was how English was pronounced in Elizabethan Britain back in the 16th century! The ships did not carry across the way the language was actually spoken!

The reality is that humans are intensely social, and will go to great lengths to identify with a larger social order, and maintain characteristics of that identity. The less contact there is, the stronger this imperative.

In  Ice Age Europe, when nomadic hunter-gatherers were sparsely spread over a vast territory, the imperative to maintain an identity – such as keeping a constant culture among a category of people, such as reindeer hunters -  was intense. When two tribes met after years of separation, both were driven to find their common characteristics again. After leaving each other, they were determined to not change, and as a result had a sense of what was ‘proper’. Change was bad, as it would remove them from their larger social order.

Each side looked for what they had in common, and emphasized it. What was different was gradually abandoned. Every meeting thus was a correction against divergence. But the ideal was to resist change in the time between meeting – which could be years. If all peoples had an ideal of speaking properly, then when they met, they would already be speaking similarly, and there would scarcely be a need for correction,

Thus, those scholars who have imagined in early Europe there were thousands of languages, are completely wrong. Humans naturally want to converge, to be similar, to speak similarly. It is the reason large scale trends develop.  While it may have been true that in southern Europe, where people did not have to roam as far to survive, the extent of a language was smaller.

When farming developed and people lived in settlements, the extent of a language became very small, since farmers did not have to travel beyond their settlement. If there was additionally overpopulation and competition. Then groups would want to polarize. There is a reason why in both the jungles of New Guinea and the city states of ancient Asia Minor, each tribe developed their own customs, costumes, and languages – to separate their identity from neighbouring peoples that were too close and competing over the same resources. The family tree model created by German philologist A. Schleicher in the late 1800äs and used by Setälä would have been applicable if the Ural Mountains area had been located among the civilizations of Asia Minor, or in New Guinea – not in the ancient hinterlands of northwest Eurasia!

While linguistic divergence may have been common in overpopulated jungles or civilizations, we are here dealing with northern seasonally nomadic hunter-gatherers in a low-food density environment. Competition was minimal or non-existent. The desire of people who rarely encountered other peoples was to unite with other peoples, not to polarize with them as would be the case in a highly populated, competitive, region.

LINGUISTIC  INTERPRETATION ARCHEOLOGICAL & POPULATION GENETICS

Towards the north, where life was still difficult and hunter-gatherers have to cover vast areas of forests, marshes, mountains or tundras, there was very little linguistic variation, for reasons described. As discussed earlier, our theory sees the changes during the rapid climate warming at around 11,000-10,000 years ago producing two major human migrations or expansions (an expansion being numerous migrations) – the expansion of European boat peoples, and the migrations of Asian reindeer peoples. I am calling the language of the original boat peoples – those arising from the archeological “Maglemose” or “Kunda” culture – as “UR-FINNIC”. (“Ur” is a prefix meaning ‘earliest, original’. I use it so it is not confused with  the established use of “Proto-“ which means ‘first, preceding’)  For the original Asian reindeer peoples I am using the word “UR-SAMOYEDIC”. The latter could be ancestral to Turkic languages as well.

The Ur-Finnic language could probably best identified with the language spoken by the archeological “Kunda” culture. It is possible the Ur-Finnic language of the Kunda Culture had not diverged much from the language of their ancestral reindeer people, specifically the “Swiderian” culture, or even earlier the language of the earlier  “Magdalenian” reindeer hunting culture.  (Considering the resistance to change in Ice Age arctic tundra nomadic hunter gatherers)

There being no other languages or people We can accept that originally there was only one Ur-Finnic language of boat people, one Ur-Samoyedic language of Asian reindeer people, and that all the subsequent languages were the consequence of both the dialectic subdivision according to how increased settling occurred, and in addition the spread of influences from the Ur-Samoyedic language, as described next.

If we think of the actual events we see  Ur-Finnic and Ur-Samoyedic meeting at a gathering place at the middle Urals. The two languages are different. This is not a normal situation where already related people gathered and generally only corrected each other’s speech.  Here the languages are too difficult to communicate well. It is too difficult for either side to fully learn the other’s language, so they will instead borrow each other’s most common words and expressions, adding them to their own Ur-Finnic or Ur-Samoyedic language. In the long run the meeting results in a modified Ur-Finnic that becomes Ur-Permic in the Kama River, and a modified Ur-Samoyedic that becomes Ur-Ugric in the Ob River.

(I have decided that it is much more believable that the Ob-Ugrians originated in the Ur-Samoyeds and borrowed the boat use, than that they were originally boat peoples. There are other reasons as well for deciding Ob-Ugrians were originally Ur-Samoyeds who adopted some of the Ur-Finnic way of life and some vocabulary.)
The Ur-Finnic, altered by Ur-Samoyedic, which I call Ur-Permic language,  was used in the Kama River, at gathering places at the junction of the Kama and Volga. In that contact location, the Ur-Permic language influenced the original Ur-Finnic dialect of the Volga peoples. And then the altered Volga language was carried up the Volga and influenced the Ur-Finnic at the Baltic. But the influence diminished with distance. In other words, if you offer a people a similar language, and those people wish to improve communication, they will keep what is common (the Ur-Finnic elements) and only add the Ur-Samoyedic elements that are common and important in communication. Let us take a modern example. If Finnish salesmen came south across the Gulf of Finland to Estonia, in order to communicate, they would feel around for words that they had in common, from both being Finnic languages, and then add common words not shared, if necessary, producing two hybrid languages – Finnstonian, and Estofinnish. If we now took Finnstonian to Karelia, a similar thing happens, creating a Finnstoniankarelian. This is how it works. There is no complete displacement of the underlining original language. The new Asian reindeer people language becomes a layer on top of the original Ur-Finnic, with diminishing strength with distance, so that at the Baltic, the Finnic languages are least ‘contaminated’ by the influences from the east, and more pure in terms of retaining the original Ur-Finnic.

By this theory, then, an illusion arose for the linguists looking at northwest Eurasian languages that there had been a divergence from a common parent, rather than two different languages being influenced by each other.

Figure 7

ALTERNATIVE MODEL WITH TWO  LANGUAGE ORIGINS & EARLY CONVERGENCE PLUS IN SITU DIALECTIC DIVERGENCE

ARROWED GREY REPRESENTS NATURAL SHIFTS, EXPANSIONS
FUSED GREYS BETWEEN OVALS REPRESENTS GROUPINGS FORMING DIALECTIC DIVISIONS
VERTICAL GREY LINES REPRESENT DESCENT OVER MILLENIA WITH NATURAL CHANGE






This diagram assumes two origins – a Ur-Finnic one near the source of the boat peoples at the Baltic, and a Ur-Samoyedic one of reindeer peoples who managed to reach the arctic with their reindeer herds. Ur-Finnic with little impact from the mixed language at the Urals evolves to modern Finnic, and Ur-Samoyedic with little impact from the mixed language at the Urals evolves into modern Samoyedic. All the rest after the creation of the mixed language impact the languages between according to travel distance from the Urals origins of the mixed language. See also another graphic description in Figure 8  next.



Figure 8
THE ALTERNATIVE MODEL DESCRIBED WITH RECTANGLES REPRESENTING ORIGINAL BROADLY DISTRIBUTED LANGUAGES IN NW EURASIA GEOGRAPHY

1. ARROWS REPRESENT EXPANSIONS OF BOAT PEOPLES, AND NORTHWARD SHIFTS OF REINDEER PEOPLES
2. DOUBLE ENDED ARROW REPRESENTS UR-SAMOYEDIC INFLUENCING UR-FINNIC AND UR-FINNIC INFLUENCING UR-SAMOYEDIC, PRODUCING UR-PERMIC AND UR-UGRIC RESPECTIVELY
3. GRAPHIC 3 REPRESENTS BOTH EVOLUTION OF THE “UR-“ LANGUAGES IN SITU, AND SOFT INFLUENCES FROM THE UR-PERMIC (WHICH BECOMES PERMIC)









RECTANGLES REPRESENTING NORTHWEST EURASIA
Graphic 1 shows the development and expansion out of European  of boat-oriented hunter-gatherers as the melting glaciers flooded the lands that were released, while Asian reindeer people  continued with reindeer, shifting north. Graphic 2 shows the contact made probably at the middle Urals, which results in their two languages, cultures, and genes mixing (thick double arrow), with a Ur-FInnic at the foundation of one and Ur-Samoyedic at the foundation of the other. The changes at the Urals diffuse west. (thin arrows) Graphic 3 shows the results, including a long term dialectic divergence influenced by the edges of the major water basins tending to reduce contacts across the water system boundaries.



4.

FINAL WORDS


SCIENCES STUDYING REAL REMAINS IN THE GROUND MUST BE AT THE CORE OF RECONSTRUCTION

A century ago, linguists were operating in a vaccuum of information. Almost nothing was known about the European past. Understandings about the Ice Age, and its peoples were vague. Archeology was new, and still only interested in ancient ruins in southeast Europe. But as the decades passed, the science of archeology grew quickly and the amount of knowledge grew.

What we know today that was not available to linguists a century ago, is astonishing. What have we learned that the creators of the “Uralic Language Family” theory did not know?

Here is a sample:
 The decline of European reindeer hunters, and rise of the “Maglemose” and “Kunda” cultures; the rate of warming as determined from remains of pollen and what it revealed about climate and environmental change; understanding of the depressed lands when glaciers melted, that flooded the lands more than they are today; the continued glacier cover over Scandinavia after the climate became as warm as today; the large glacial water seas that blocked the migration of reindeer, but permitted the expansion of aquatic life and people who invented dugouts to harvest it;…..

The amount of information that has accumulated is enormous, and this short paper could be expanded ten times. The information is readily found. But there is little organization of the information around the theme of the past of northwest Eurasia.

There is plenty of information about the expansions of boat peoples (see Paabo:Uirala) at the end of the Ice Age. Less known is the story of the Asian reindeer peoples, and their history.  Is there more evidence of a contact location near the middle Urals, as described? When and where did some of the reindeer peoples abandon their original pedestrian way of life hunting reindeer, or even managing them in semi-domestication?  Did  the migation through the Urals result in those reaching the arctic continue their reindeer-oriented way of life, while their brothers lagging behind decided to continue hunting diverse animals, and, using boats expand also into hunting and gathering aquatic animals and plants?

 It seems common sense that if reindeer numbers declined, its hunters would turn to other animals, and be amazed to see arriving boat peoples accessing flooded lands with boats and living on fish. It is also concievable that the most southerly of reindeer people who abandoned their original way of life, not only became involved with boat peoples, but carried on reindeer-oriented skills to horse herds.  Hungarians do not have to have originated close to the source of the Ob River, but could have originated with contact with boat peoples at the south end of the Urals, where there is interesting archeology to be found as well.

The story of the boat peoples expanding from Europe is clear. The mystery remains with the peoples to the east of the Urals. Why is the Hungarian language closer to Turkic than Finnic? Why are surviving reindeer peoples in northern Mongolian and southern Siberian mountains, considered to speak Turkic languages. The Turkic language have an important connection to the reindeer peoples. Furthermore, Finnish speakers find a remarkable familiarity with Turkish. When we consider the migration of the N1c1 haplogroup to northern Finland, it seems that the migration brought a Turkic-like language.

When we consider all the languages traditionally called “Uralic” are we dealing with various degrees of mixing of an Ur-Finnic and Ur-Samoyedic where Ur-Samoyedic may be the ultimate origins of the Turkic languages.
 
THE INTERPRETATION THAT FITS ALL INFORMATION AVAILABLE TODAY

Because this approach begins with two languages – Ur-Finnic and Ur-Samoyedic – coming in contact with each other as early as 10,000 years ago, right away  we are dealing with early convergence. The resulting language then spreads down the Kama and influences the Volgic dialect of Ur-Finnic changing the language a little, and then the slightly changed Volgic exerts influence up the Volga into the Finnic regions, but the influence is weak. Later there is also influence of Finnic from the north.

But the least recognized mechanism is in situ dialectic divergence. See maps given in Appendix 1
The following summarizes the events that are described graphically in figures 7,8

a)expansion of European boat peoples, and northward shifting of Asian reindeer hunters;

b) contact between them occurring probably originally in the Middle Urals;

c) mixed languages arising on the west side mostly Ur-Finnic and on the east side mostly Ur-Samoyedic, producing Ur-Permic and Ur-Ugric respectively

d) the conversion of some tribes to the boat-oriented way of life, and their making Ur-Samoyedic, and N1c1-haplgroup influences into the original Ur-Finnic boat peoples to the west, perhaps facilitated by the fur trade at about 5000 years ago;

e)throughout this time the existence of natural in situ dialectic divergence in the Ur-Finnic influenced or not from the east, according to water systerm boundaries;

f) over time ending of nomadism and tighter dialectic subdivision leading to the modern languages.



=========================


Appendix 1


THE ALGONQUIAN BOAT PEOPLES OF NORTHEAST NORTH AMERICA AS EXAMPLES OF A BROAD LANGUAGE ORIGINS THAT DIVERGED  DIALECTICALLY ACCORDING TO THE NATURAL BOUNDARIES OF WATER SYSTEMS.

The Algonquian cultures of native North America are those made famous with the birchbark canoe. If we are speaking of those Algonquian tribes who were located towards the north, we find a people almost identical in way of life to the “Maglemose” and “Kunda” culture that developed at the rapid warming period at the end of the Ice Age. Perhaps the Algonquian tribes too expanded into the flooded post-glacial lands about the same time, but in North America they did not experience change, and remained close to how they originally were until European colonization. The similarities are remarkable. The only real difference is that the Algonquians developed the birchbark canoe, but towards the south, where there were no birch trees, and Algonquians further south had dugouts too. Was this a parallel development, or did knowledge cross the North Atlantic?

The traditional notion of a ‘language family’ is based on the way parents give birth to children who go off into their own world. In reality most of the world became inhabited in the Ice Age, and then all linguistic history has been about dialectic divergence within the orginally inhabited regions as the population grows, and needs to subdivide in tribal units, and also as dictated by geographical boundaries, such as seasonally nomadic boat peoples becoming divided according to water system boundaries.

The next map (Figure A2) is intended to show just how closely the dialects of the Algonquian similar languages and cultures were defined by  the water geography. Figure A2 is drawn on top of a government water drainage map, drawing lines around the water basin. I then added historically identified peoples, which suggest regions of associated clans and tribes, and their common culture and language.

Figure A1



The dialectic division of the Algonquian boat oriented (birchbark canoes) seasonally nomadic hunter-gatherers, was shaped by the water geography


Figure A2




EXAMPLE OF  LANGUAGE DISTRIBUTION OVER A BROAD GEOGRAPHY
THE IN SITU DIALECTIC SUBDIVISION OF ALGONQUIAN BOAT PEOPLES ACCORDING TO WATER BASINS IN  EASTERN CANADA

Water basins are shown by the added lines. Like in the Ur-Finnic cultures, the social and political organization of all the Algonquian (canoe-using) boat peoples were determined by the natural heirarchy of water systems. The social and political units ranged from extended families, to tribes made up of 5-6 families in a river system, and several tribes in a larger system formed a ‘nation’ and all people of a similar language was a ‘people’  We are interested in the fact that the Cree language formed a single language with only dialectic variation, that covers about the same distance as the distance between the Baltic and the Urals, thus proving that it is possible to have very broad origins, that subdivide dialectically over time, some dialects becoming extreme – ie languages. As described in the main text, while the ancestors of the Algonquians were not impacted by new developments until the recent arrival of European colonists, in northwest Eurasia, the influences began as early as 5,000 years ago with fur trade, and then farming settlements in suitable locations. This shrunk the scale of activity and subdivided the original broad foundation


============================

Appendix 2


ERRONEOUS BELIEFS OF TRADITIONAL LINGUISTS

The following are some of the mistakened beliefs I have found among traditional linguists, which I have encountered when investigating this subject. It is not unusual for fields of science to hold onto ideas that are old  but entrenched and difficult to displace. The Figure below presents a modern version of the original theory when almost nothing was known about the early circumstances in northwest Eurasia, nor even linguistics itself. The linguists simply adopted a new methodology and proceeded in the vaccuum of information of the day. It is a model that today could be created by a schoolboy or schoolgirl. It is about as antique as the early belief that the earth was flat. And yet there are still some misguided linguists who still follow it and defend it

Figure A2-1
RECTANGLES REPRESENT ASSUMED INTERMEDIATE STAGES
ARROWS REPRESENT MIGRATION-DIVERGENCE




The traditional model was created in the 1800’s when the world was obsessed with family trees of species, offspring separating from parents and changing in their new environments – producing subspecies.


In 2017 I had a lengthy email debate with a hardcore defender of the archaic theory, and learned a great deal of how they justified their position in continuing to follow it. The following are some of the beliefs, some obviously ridiculous and defensive, and my responses to them.

1. THE BELIEF THAT LANGUAGES MUST EXIST AS INDEPENDENT ARTIFACTS?

Today there are thousands of languages, but observers – especially past linguists who have studied the large numbers of languages in North America at the time of European contact – have noted that similar languages sometimes differ slightly and are dialects to each other, and sometimes are so different that it is difficult for one to understand the other. This is the result of the fact that languages are not distinct tools, artifacts, with an independent existence, but that languages in reality exists in a fluid state, often part of a continuum of changing – converging in some ways, diverging in others, from their prior form or from nearby related languages. Humans in the natural state will generate a continuum based on a single language that changes dialectically in such a way that nearby dialects are close to each other, while dialects far away can be so different as to be regarded as related languages rather than dialects. In the natural state, therefore, there is a single language with dialects developed from patterns of contact – much contact causing dialects to resist change, little contact allowing the dialects to be more easily changed.

What we know as “languages” are the consequence of dramatic changes due to some external factors causing those dramatic changes in a continuum. The most common natural factors causing the dramatic changes, and even sharp discontinuities, were geographic barriers or boundaries. For example, originally there was a continuum of a single Finnic language going up the east Baltic coast, that varied dialectically in the fashion described above, but when it came to the Gulf of Finland, there was a sharp discontinuity caused by the Gulf of Finland geographic barrier. Of course to some extent, there was still some continuity with dialects going around the east end of the Gulf of Finland.  Another example is that we must regard the Ural Mountains as a barrier causing both sides to developing linguistically a little differently even if both languages developed from the same circumstances of contact between the Ur-Finnic and Ur-Samoyedic languages. I have also above stressed that for boat-oriented nomadic peoples, the boundaries of the inhabited water system form soft barriers to communication that caused the dialects in one language to be different from the dialects in another language.

But MOST languages today are not the result of natural barriers, but the result of creation of artificial nations, and the imposition of national languages on governed regions that originally contained the contiinuum of a single language. And that national language was based on the language of the strongest group.

That means when linguistics analyzes modern national languages, defined by arbitrary national boundaries, it is not dealing with language in a pure way, and as a result the results are somewhat contrived. Significantly when linguistics of Finnic languages jumps from Estonian to Livonian to Finnish to Karelian, and so on, it is not seeing the continuum, but leaping between languages that for one reason or another have been politically allowed to survive. All the dialects that originally existed between those surviving languages are treated as if they never existed.

But linguistics would be unable to analyze a continuum. Imagine that the modern Algonquian linguistic divisions created by water system boundaries, did not exist. There would be a single Algonquian language continuum, changing smoothly from tribe to tribe (we can allow minor steps of change from tribe to tribe), from the Atlantic to the middle of Canada.

This is very important, because comparative linguistics would not work on a continuum. Comparative linguistics has to deal with situations in which an original language breaks up into two strong dialects which then generates offsprings up to the modern day. Linguistics can only compare surviving descendants of the original dialectic divergence.

Linguistics can only compare surviving languages. It therefore cannot identify descendants of dialects when they did not produce descendant languages that reached the modern day. For example if there once existed Finnic languages say in the aboriginal peoples of Scandinavia, comparative linguistics cannot see it. Insofar as comparative linguistics today does not even acknowledge the possibility of extinct languages, it is unlikely to present a true picture of language evolution.

If we went back to the Ice Age, we would tend to find single languages spread practically endlessly in a continuum. Discontinuities could be caused by geographical barriers.. Common sense suggests that there would have been some natural barriers too in terms of how peoples following one way of life (horse hunters) occupied a different environment than peoples following another way of life (say, reindeer hunters) But there would be underlying continuity where an ancestral people diverged between those who went in two different directions.

Thus all languages are not of the same nature and we have to be restrained in what we conclude from them, while others may still reflect a natural continuum among aboriginal peoples.

2. THE BELIEF THAT LANGUAGES HAVE TIGHT ORIGINS

This is the silliest claim by traditional “Uralic” linguists who want to find an original “Uralic” parent language near the Ural Mountains. This silly idea arises only when linguistics assumes language evolves from a group splitting off from a parent language and migrating away. Any theory that involves migrating away, requres that the parent language has a tight origin, If the language were widely distributed over an entire water system, then moving away would require travelling close to 1000km, to enter a new water system, since everyone still within a water system would continue to have contact with the original language. Obviously if we are dealing with highly nomadic peoples, the correct mechanism for linguistic divergence is for the original broad region of a single language, to subdivide dialectically for various reasons. Since the geography is constant, the dialectic divergence will be caused by natural continued divergence in the existing geography, or from the inhabitants changing their way of life so at to reduce contact throughout the original broad area. For example if members of a tribe no longfer travelled the entire river system, but reduced their nomadic ways to one branch of the river system, then dialectic divergence would occur within that original complete river system.

It is important to note that when the region of a single language with dialectic divergence (ie a continuum with some geographical influences developing dialectic divergence) is not static.  If we have nomadic peoples maintaining a single language, over time, the language of the whole region will evolve. What is constant is that there is uniformity over a broad area, and then that subsequent contraction of that broad area by further internal dialectic divergence, occurred to that broad language at that time.

In other words, if we accept that there was once an Ur-Finnic language that spread east as far as the Urals, we cannot claim that any modern Finnic language looks like the Ur-Finnic language. However, the larger the region of a single language, the more mobile the peple maintaining that language was,  the more inertia there would be in resisting change in one location or another.  Today we see the inertia in English, where the amount of use of English causes resistance to change. On the other hand, a local dialect in a community somewhere can quickly change, as it has little inertia.

Thus to conclude – traditonal “Uralic” linguists have no choice but to claim a tight origins near the Urals if they hold onto the idea of divergence arising from migrations. A century ago, the very idea of divergence by dialectic subdivision, was not acknowledged.

3. THE BELIEF THAT THE SPREAD OF THE TRADITIONAL “URALIC” LANGUAGE EITHER INVOLVED ENTRY INTO EMPTY LANDS AND/OR COMPLETELY DISPLACED THE ORGINAL LANGUAGE

This idea is also necessary to defend the original “Uralic Language Family” idea, in the face of increasing knowledge from archeology about the expansion of humankind out of Ice Age Europe, a general west-to-east expansion. Since the traditional “Uralic” theory originally spoke about origins at the Urals around 4000 years ago, and an arrival at the Baltic by 2000 years ago, archeological discoveries forced the need to consider who were there at the Baltic BEFORE 2000 years ago. Scholars were confused, but generally assumed they were the peoples historians called “Finns” or “Fenni” – people related to the modern Saami (formerly “Lapps” and before that “Finns”). So the scholar had to assume that the modern Finnic peoples, notably Finns, were not descended from original people because they came from the east. But the Saami language is close enough to Finnish to be considered related to it.

So there existed a ridiculous situation. Theories that the arriving Finns from the east influenced the Saami language. To summarize a whole century of head-scratching, no evidence was found of an incoming people from the east, displacing or even influencing any native people. When archeology discovered that a “Comb-ceramic” material culture appeared in the entire east Baltic with pottery styles that appeared to have come from the upper Volga. This was enough for linguists to claim this was the time of the migration. They only needed to push back the arrival at the Baltic from 2000 years ago to 5,000 years ago! Archeologists (ie Richard Indreko) dismissed the logic, saying the influence from the upper Volga was purely a movement of a cultural feature, not an entire tribe.  There is evidence that long distance trade, probably fur trade, began at that time. This is confirmed by the spread of distribution of amber objects throughout the “Comb-ceramic” area and also in ancient Babylonian tombs,

There was a spread of an influence up the Volga, but you do not require an entire people to migrate to move something that more likely was carried by small groups of traders, or simply carried by families to gathering places.

It is also impossible to displace an original language with a new language, unless the new language is very strong, expecially carried by conquerors who kill off the indigenous peoples and force the new language on the indigenous people. Something like that occurred in the colonization of North America – hundreds of languages were wiped out. But always in history, there will be remote places where the original language endures. In the case of North America, languages have survived, especially in the north. In the British Isles, the Pictish language survived in the north until around the 10th century. It is possible to think of the Saami language as a remnant of languages that once dominated all of the Scandinavian Peninsula. Across Eurasia, original languages can be found that have not been replaced.

Even in these cases of replacement, the new language is backed by major powers. In order to argue an “Uralic” language completely replacing the original language (the Ur-Finnic) we need to find the language being spread by very powerful people bent on conquest and domination. History shows nothing.

Therefore if there is evidence in Finnic languages of origins that point back east as far as reindeer people, then it is more likely the original Ur-Finnic language was simply influenced by a language that was already Ur-Finnic at its foundations. Below we will propose that the Ur-Finnic peoples who reached the Urals, adopted aspects of the language of the reindeer people speaking Ur-Samoyedic, and the resulting language, which I am calling “Ur-Permic” then began to exert influences back east, first Ur-Permic on the original Ur-Finnic Volgic dialect, and then the slightly changed Volgic dialect in turn influencing the Ur-Finnic near the Baltic. The influences could indeed spread in step by step through boat peoples according to the major water systems. These influences would be superimposed onto the original and continuing internal dialectic divergence, resulting in the founding languages of the later development of the descendant languages that survived to the modern day.

However, a century ago, linguists wanted desparately to discover a linguistic family tree, and to use the migrations approach, and employ the new methodology, and there was no consideration of the simple spread  of linguistic influences in diminishing degrees with distance. ( Volgic  fur traders could have been the instruments of the spread of influences.).

4. THE BELIEF THAT INFLUENCES – SUCH AS BORROWINGS – CAN BE IGNORED BY LINGUISTS OR CAN BE DETECTED BY LACK OF COGNATES

One of the claims made by a traditional “Uralic” linguist was that a linguist cannot confuse similarity from convergence of two languages for similarity from divergence from a common parent.  The reason, was the claim, is that convergence is about borrowing and borrowed words can be detected from lack of cognates. Original words in a language, eventually develop words of related form and meaning, that are known as cognates. New words borrowed from other languages will not have existed in the language long enough to develop a history that leads to speakers developing cognates from them.

However, if a borrowed word is very popular, then it will become part of the language more quickly. But most of all, if the borrowing occurred a long time ago, then it would behave like any other original word, and its borrowed origin would not be detectable. As we see in these pages, the contact between the Ur-Finnic boat peoples and the Ur-Samoyedic reindeer people occurred as early as about 10,000 years ago. That is 5,000 year before the linguistic analysis using only the migration-divergence approach, suggests. What we have here – if you follow the story told by archeology and population genetics – is borrowing that took place so long ago that languages that developed from convergence between Ur-Finnic and Ur-Samoyedic will show similarities that can be erroneously interpreted as divergence from a “Uralic” original language, rather than an early convergence between Ur-Finnic and Ur-Samoyedic as early as 10,000 years ago. (Even some millenia later, there will still be enough time for the evidence of the original borrowing to be faded.)

It is therefore easily possible that traditional linguists mistook the convergence between two original languages to be a divergence from a hypothetical parent language.

Traditional historical comparative linguistics is now obsolete because most languages evolved by convergence – often going back to the end of the Ice Age – and yet comparative linguistic methodology is unable to detect or analyze convergence

5. THE BELIEF THAT LANGUAGE, CULTURE, AND GENETICS ARE INDEPENDENT AND ONE HAS NO BEARING ON THE OTHER

Today, we can find many examples of the independence of language used, culture (way of life)  and genetics. For example we could have a black skinned African, speaking Finnish, and practicing, say, Japaneses culture.

But in the beginning – we have to realize – language originated as a tool that referred to the real world with symbols in order to communicate information. Reindeer people needed to have words for reindeer, their gender, the landscape, the spear, how to throw spears at reindeer, etc.  Thus in the beginning a language mirrored the way of life. Language and culture were intimately connected.

However when reindeer people adapted to using boats to hunt seals in the sea, the words for reindeer, and hunting reindeer on the tundra were useless. Since no language is created entirely anew, what happens is that the useless words are rarely used and forgotten. On the other hand a new array of words was necessary to reflect, the new way of life. New words were necessary for boat, the sea, seals, etc, and were invented, often from new uses for old words. Could a seal be referred to as a ‘water-reindeer’?  The harpoon used to hunt seals could be called ‘spear’.

Many words could be kept – words for sun, earth, land, sky, family relations like ‘mother’ ‘father’, ‘family group’. Thus if a people inherits a language but changes their way of life, there can be much continuity in the original language. A language is more likely to shift the meaning of existing words to suit new similar circumstances than invent a completely new word that never existed before.

Now, regardng genetics. We know from the behaviour of our closest relatives, the apes, that males forming groups are rulers and defenders of territory. New males grow up and are included in the society of males. There is a chief, a leader, among the males, and he is challenged by younger males who wish to take over. Humans come from the same place – males take charge, compete for leadership, and as a whole defend the tribe against predators or rival tribes. Indeed we can look beyond the apes to animals that encircle females and young against an advancing pack of wolves.

That means in the human past, males passed down the role of defender of the tribe generation after generation. Males never departed from their tribe They could lead the creation of a new tribe, but If a tribe existed, its males would never leave. They would fetch their wives from neighbouring tribes. Wives would be brought home to live beside their mothers. ‘

For this reason the population genetics DNA marker passed down from father to son, is revealing. It generally shows that the apparent migration of a male haplogroup, was in fact the migration of the tribe they belonged to. Haplogroups pertaining to females do not reveal the movement of tribes, because the males might obtain their wives from far away. Furthermore the daughter of the wife might then be again taken far away into another tribe. Hence DNA in female lineage only reveal a blurry sense of the range of a tribe over a long period. Female DNA is most useful for permanently settled peoples where males did not go very far to find their wives.

Thus genetics of the male lineage help us trace the migration of tribes from the apparent migrations of their haplogroup marker.  If we can now identify the cultural development of the tribe, we can also link the male haplogroup to culture. For example on these pages we can determine that the N-haplogroup was originally in males of reindeer people, therefore when this haplogroup appears in boat people, we can infer that there were times when tribes converted from reindeer people way of life to boat people way of life, and then from then on the N-haplogroup was spread via male offspring, through the boat people world, radiating from the original source location.

In terms of language, we use common sense. If reindeer people change to boat people, and interract with them, they will have to learn the language of the boat people to the degree of involvement. Language is not independent of circumstances involving way or life and genetics. Mixed marriages of course influence the use of a mixed languages, It raises the use of the mixed language which then exerts a stronger influence on unmixed dialects.

In general, all three parameters – language, culture, and genetics – although independent, interract with one another in the real world, and it is necessary to reconstruct the actual events before forming conclusions.
Nothing is achieved by pursing each of these parameters independently of the others since the nature of influences varies with total circumstances.

6. THE BELIEF THAT THERE IS NO LINGUISTIC CONTINUITY AND THAT LANGUAGES ARE CONSTANTLY CHANGING

Since no language is developed from scratch, all language has some degree of continuity from a language that came before and formed the foundation for the new language. However, there could be truth that in advanced civilizations, the language of a dominant people can displace the language of a weaker people, and that such events can occur alongside historical events, where one conquering people is later conquered by another, For example, Britain was conquered by the Romans, and most of it converted to Latin, but then five centuries later, Britain was conquered by Germanic Saxons and Romans. But this kind of behaviour is possible only in warring, competing, powers in civilizations. In the case of the early languages of northwest Eurasia, there is no evidence of any such conquering people or anyone able to displace an original language. The fact that Finnic languages are filled with imagery of marshy lands and boat use, do not suggest the language came from another culture than the original Ur-Finnic of the orignal expansion of boat people. But it is possible for Finnic to be the consequence of being slightly changed by influences from the east and north that originated in the early Ur-Samoyedic.

When dealing with aboriginal peoples we cannot claim any full replacement of an original language by a new one, especially if there aren’t even any examples of remnants of the original language in remote places.
 


REFERENCES

The purpose of this article was to reinterpret the “Uralic Language Family” theory  after a century of accumulated knowledge in archeology, and other applicable sciences. This re-interpretation is entirely original in this article

Clark, G,   1967  World Prehistory, Cambridge   A celebrated text that summarized the accumulated archeological discoveries up to that time. Since then the ideas have simply been refined.
Jaanits, L. et al, 1982, Eesti Esiakalugu, Eesti Raamat, Tallinn  In Estonian, the product of Estonian archeological work during the Soviet period, where the authors were able to access the work of other archeology within the Soviet Union, not as accessible in the west.
Kozlowski J, and Bandi H-G  1984  The Paleohistory of Circumpolar Arctic Colonization, Arctic 37 (4): 359-372  Article in English, where the investigation of the northeast Europe and the Urals was only one section. I chose to use it for reference because of this focus, and because it was a summary.
 Pääbo, Andres  2002-2016  WEBSITE: The Origins and Expansions of the Ancient Boat-oriented Way of Life: Basic Introduction to the Theory of a Worldwide Expansion of Boat-peoples from Northern Europe
 , [http://www.paabo.ca/uirala/ui-ra-la.html]      This is currently a layman-type site, not scholarly, initially created for fun beginning 1998;; however the content contains much that is original new theory from more or less raw data.
Rootsi,S., et al. 2006, A counterclockwise northern route of the Y-chromosome haplogroup N from Southeast Asia towards Europe”  European Journal of Human Genetics 15 (2): 204-11  Comment: This is regarded as the authorative study suggesting the N1c1 haplogroup migrated up the Ural Mountains and then continued west along the arctic coast of northeast Europe to the northern Finland area, and then diffused into the Finno-Ugric speakers from the locations of the reindeer peoples. This agrees with the other paleoclimatological and other facts. This association proves something that should be obvious from the archeological/climatological story – that the Samoyedic reindeer peoples were of Asian origins, not European.




 <<<BACK

<<< GO TO UIRALA CONTENTS PAGE

author: A.Paabo, Box 478, Apsley, Ont., Canada

 

2018 (c) A. Pääbo.